view counter

Social networksSocial networking makes us smarter now, but more stupid in the long run

Published 7 February 2014

Does improved connectivity to other people through social networks makes us smarter or more stupid? Some say that connectivity allows us acquire information from other people as well as by direct experience. Many pundits say that in the Internet era, in which we have access to a diversity of information, humankind will learn to make more informed decisions. Others, however, suggest having so much information at our fingertips will limit our ability for concentration, contemplation, and reflection. Controlled tests show that both arguments are correct – but on different tike scales: Being able to copy from other people in vast networks means analytical responses rapidly spread – but only by making it easy and commonplace for people to reach analytical response without engaging analytical processing. The researchers conclude that this tendency to copy without thinking “can explain why increased connectivity may eventually make us stupid by making us smarter first.”

Social media provide rapid transfer of information at the cost of analytical thought // Source: wm.edu

In a study published in the Journal of the Royal Society Interface scientists have found that whilst mass connectivity through social media and the Internet makes us look smarter, it might be making us stupider.

Copying other people has helped the human race become adaptable, letting us acquire information from other people as well as by direct experience. In the Internet era, in which we have access to a diversity of information, many pundits say humankind will learn to make more informed decisions.

Others, however, suggest having so much information at our fingertips will limit our ability for concentration, contemplation, and reflection. A Royal Society release reports that a team of scientists, seeking to find out whether social networks make us smarter, investigated whether networks help us imitate analytical thought processes from our peers.

To carry out their experiment the researchers tested university students with a series of brain-straining questions. One-hundred volunteers were separated into five social networks, each with twenty individuals. Connections between the people in the networks were assigned randomly by a computer to fit five different network patterns. At one extreme, all the people in the network were connected directly to all the others, and at the other extreme there were no connections at all. To test how these networks helped the people in them to learn, the scientists quizzed the volunteers with a “cognitive reflection test,” a series of questions which rely on analytical reasoning to overcome incorrect intuition.

To see whether the social networks helped the people in them to improve their answers, the volunteers were asked each of the questions five times. The first time the volunteers had to figure it out on their own, the next five times they were allowed to copy the answer from their neighbors in the network. The researchers found that in well-connected networks volunteers copy-cats got better at giving the right answer the more times they were asked and the more opportunities they had to steal their neighbors’ answers. This result showed that when the students had lots of connections to peers they could recognize where they had given a wrong answer and swap it for the right one, proving to the scientists that well-connected networks can help us get the right answer because we can copy from our peers.

The scientists compared how well the volunteers faired in the three consecutive questions to see whether the volunteers were actually getting better at figuring out the problems themselves or just at copying the right answers. They found that there was no improvement from one question to the next; even when individuals had realized in the first round of questions that finding the solutions required deeper thought, in the next question they were back at square one. The scientists say it is a surprising result because it has already been proven that analytical reasoning can be primed very simply, by showing subjects a picture of Rodin’s Thinker for example, or using a challenging font to type up questions. The team says the results show that whilst social networks helped the volunteers choose better answers, they did not prime them to answer more logically themselves, showing that “social learning does not seem to help individuals bypass their bias in favor of intuition but rather help society as a whole thrive despite this bias.”

The release notes that whilst some commentators say the Internet is making us stupid and others say it is helping us make more informed decisions, the scientists behind this study say that they might both be right. Being able to copy from other people in vast networks means analytical responses rapidly spread, fulfilling their promise of improved decision-making for well-connected people. “On the other hand, the bias may very well decrease the frequency of analytical reasoning by making it easy and commonplace for people to reach analytical response without engaging analytical processing,” says the team, and this tendency to copy without thinking “can explain why increased connectivity may eventually make us stupid by making us smarter first.”

— Read more in Iyad Rahwan et al., “Analytical reasoning task reveals limits of social learning in networks,” Journal of the Royal Society Interface 11, no. 93 (6 February 2014) (doi: 10.1098/rsif.2013.1211)

view counter
view counter